Is homosexuality a born instinct?

Paul Billings & Jonathan Beckwith

July 20, 1993|By Paul Billings & Jonathan Beckwith

In an era when researchers have located the genes for conditions such as Huntington's disease, cystic fibrosis, and Duchenne muscular dystrophy, some scientists believe that "homosexual" genes will soon be found.

It comes as no surprise, then, that lately the popular press has been humming with stories about whether homosexuality is genetically determined. We are uneasy about the current unbridled enthusiasm for studies relating genes with human behavior.

Scientists' arguments for a biological basis for human differences have previously been used for insidious ends; the arguments by German scientists before World War II for the genetic inferiority of Jews is just one example.

Moreover, much of the older scientific analysis of the origins of human behavior, particularly using biological methods, has been debunked.

In the nineteenth century, for instance, "phrenologists" claimed that they could predict aspects of an individual's personality, such as sexuality, intelligence, and criminal tendencies, merely by examining the skull's structure. Despite its popularity, this "science," which often included explicitly racist implications, was not based on any reliable evidence.

More recently, after studies in prisons in the 1960s, geneticists jumped to the conclusion that males with an extra Y chromosome were more likely to be criminals than other men. Follow-up studies in the general population showed that this claim was unwarranted.

But that is the past. It's always possible that the field of human behavioral genetics has shaken off its tawdry history.

Today, when sophisticated techniques can be used to analyze human DNA, maybe the renewed interest in connecting biology and behavior portends the development of a more scientific era. On the other hand, maybe not.

A look at recent studies seeking a genetic basis for homosexuality suggests that many of the problems of the past have recurred. We may be in for a new molecular phrenology rather than true scientific progress and insight into behavior.

The first issue to reckon with when conducting studies of homosexuality or other behavior entails developing a clear-cut picture of who exhibits the trait and who doesn't.

This "labeling" problem is exemplified in recent research of Simon LeVay, a respected neuroanatomist. While at the Salk Institute in La Jolla, Calif., LeVay did postmortem examinations of the brains of primarily young male victims of AIDS.

In a study published in Science in 1991, he reported a difference between heterosexual and homosexual males in the size of the hypothalamus. But he could not really be certain about his subjects' sexual preferences, since they were dead.

Also, the research design and subject sample did not allow others to determine whether it was sexual behavior, drug use, or disease history that was correlated with the observed differences among the subjects' brains.

Even for the living, scientists must decide whether, when trying to classify a person as gay, to rely on the frequency of homosexual behavior, the age at which it began, or, as some psychiatrists have suggested, the presence of homoerotic fantasies.

Compounding this difficulty is the reluctance of many gay people to admit their sexual preference. In this situation one can easily create inaccurate or inconsistent study groups.

A related problem cropped up in a study by Michael Bailey, a psychologist at Northwestern University, and Richard Pillard, a psychiatrist at Boston University School of Medicine.

In research published in 1991 in Archives of General Psychiatry, they reported that among identical twins reared together, each individual had a high probability (52 percent) of being homosexual if the other twin was.

For non-identical twins, adopted brothers, and non-twin siblings, the correlations were much lower (22, 11, and 9 percent, respectively).

The researchers solicited participation for their study through advertisements in gay newspapers. But this could well have produced a biased sample.

It is not unreasonable to think that identical twins who are both openly homosexual would be more likely to volunteer for such a study because of the shared knowledge that they are both comfortable with their sexuality. Furthermore, if one gay twin "comes out," it might be easier for the other to do the same.

While the authors interpreted their findings as evidence for a genetic basis for homosexuality, we think that the data in fact provide strong evidence for the influence of the environment.

On average, both non-identical twins and non-twin siblings share 50 percent of their genes. If homosexuality were a genetic trait, the pairs in these groups should be homosexual a similar percentage of the time. They certainly should be homosexual more often than adopted siblings.

Baltimore Sun Articles
|
|
|
Please note the green-lined linked article text has been applied commercially without any involvement from our newsroom editors, reporters or any other editorial staff.